Digital Number Line Games Improve Number Sense and Basic Arithmetic
Skills Among Young Children
Author: Chien-Heng Lin (Department of Early Childhood Education, National Dong Hwa University)
Vol.&No.:Vol. 68, No. 2
Date:June 2023
Pages:73-101
DOI:https://doi.org/10.6209/JORIES.202306_68(2).0003
Abstract:
Background and Purpose
The main purpose of teaching number sense to young children is to establish a core knowledge framework for number concepts, thereby enabling young children to understand a wide range of number problems and providing a basis for children to learn more complex number concepts in the future. Number sense is also fundamental for learning formal mathematics later in life. Studies have associated a lack of number sense in early childhood with a subsequent decline and difficulty in mathematical learning. Therefore, cultivating number sense in early childhood is essential for future mathematics achievement.
The development of young children’s numerical cognition and mathematical skills is closely linked to the establishment of a mental number line. Additionally, studies have discovered a strong correlation between an individual’s innate perception of the mental number line and their proficiency in a range of mathematical abilities, including understanding number concepts, estimating quantities, and performing arithmetic.
With the goal of developing young children’s number sense and basic arithmetic skills, this study designed a set of targeted intervention tools in the form of digital number line games. These tools were designed to help young children develop an understanding of mental number lines, thereby improving their number sense and basic arithmetic skills. This study also compared the improvement effects of the number line game and a non-number line game on the aforementioned learning objectives to determine which game was more effective at improving the relevant learning outcomes.
Literature Review
The meaning of number sense
Number sense can be described as an individual’s ability to understand, relate, and connect numbers. Children with number sense can understand the relevant concepts and principles of numbers. Number sense includes several mathematical aspects, such as the ability for estimating small quantities, counting and cardinality, comparing numbers, recognizing number patterns, and transforming numbers. The concept of number sense in early childhood refers not to a single mathematical ability but rather encompasses a set of essential cognitive components that aid in comprehending numbers and abstract numerical symbols. These components include a conceptual understanding and the ability to perform practical numerical operations.
Development of a mental number line and the relationship between a mental number line and number sense in children
The ability to place numbers on a mental number line is associated with the development of number sense, which is the ability to understand, relate, and connect numbers. Relevant studies have suggested that an individual’s perception of the mental number line influences their development of several number sense components (e.g., estimating, counting, and transforming numbers). Therefore, as soon as young children are capable of counting, connecting, and estimating the size of numbers and have learnt the numeral system and the spatial arrangement of numbers, they may acquire the ability to perceive a mental number line. Through educational activities, children can learn how to spatially arrange numbers (i.e., visualize a mental number line) and effectively develop a complete understanding of advance number concepts, including number transformation.
Relationship between the construction of a mental number line and basic arithmetic
The construction of a mental number line involves spatially arranging numbers by size in the mind. Numbers are arranged in a continuous, quantity-based analogical format in ascending order (from left to right). When constructing a mental number line, basic arithmetic is performed. For example, the number after 6 is 7. To arrive at 7, addition arithmetic (6 + 1) must be performed. Therefore, the construction of a mental number line plays an important role in the development of basic arithmetic skills.
Methods
This study employed an experimental research design. A total of 105 preschool children from rural areas in eastern Taiwan were enrolled into this study. Participants were randomly divided into three groups: two experimental groups and a control group. A pretest-posttest analysis was performed to explore the influence of number line games on number sense and basic arithmetic skills. One-way analysis of covariance was performed to determine whether significant differences were present between the pretest and posttest results and between the three groups to eliminate the influence of confounding variables.
Results
Results for the number sense
Covariate analysis revealed significant differences in posttest scores between the three groups (F(2,101) = 64.47, p < .05, eta squared η2 = .56). Furthermore, the improvement in number sense in the two experimental groups was significant. Both interventions (a number line game and a non-number line game) significantly improved the number sense to a similar extent.
Results for basic arithmetic skills
Significant differences were observed in the improvement of addition and subtraction skills between the two experimental groups and the control group. Basic arithmetic skills were better in the two experimental groups than in the control group. Additionally, the group that played the number line game performed significantly better than the group that played the non-number line game.
Conclusion
This research is one of the few studies that has explored the development of number sense among young children by using a number line game. The findings indicate that the number line game, which was designed in accordance with the mental number line concept, can effectively enhance number sense among young children. However, in terms of improving number sense, similar learning outcomes can also be achieved by using non-number line games. In terms of improving basic arithmetic skills (addition and subtraction), the number line game was more effective than the non-number line game (traditional number decomposition and synthesis game). Furthermore, the non-number line game was more effective than the intervention in the control group. Overall, the digital game intervention aimed at improving mathematical learning among young children (whether it involves a number line game or a traditional non-number line game) had a positive effect on improving numerical ability, including number sense and basic arithmetic. The number line game was more effective than the non-number line game and the intervention in the control group. These findings are expected to advance the theoretical literature of mathematics education in early childhood and the teaching practices of mathematics for young children.
Keywords:young children’s mathematics, basic arithmetic ability, number line game, number sense
《Full Text》
References:
- 王國亨、簡清華(2008)。屏東縣國小一年級新生的數與計算能力。教育實踐與研究,21(2),1-31。https://doi.org/10.6776/JEPR.200809.0001【Wang, K.-H., & Chien, C.-H. (2008). First graders’ abilities of number and operations in Pingtung County. Journal of Educational Practice and Research, 21(2), 1-31. https://doi.org/10.6776/JEPR.200809.0001】
- 杜雪淇、阮淑宜、林珮伃(2011)。弱勢家庭與非弱勢家庭大班幼兒數學能力之研究。幼兒教育年刊,22,21-41。【Du, S.-C., Juan, S.-Y., & Lin, P.-Y. (2011). Kindergarteners’ mathematics abilities between disadvantaged and advantaged families. Journal of Early Childhood Education, 22, 21-41.】
- 周姍姍、王馨敏(2021)。幼兒視覺空間工作記憶與翻牌遊戲之關聯性研究。教育科學研究期刊,66(3),191-212。https://doi.org/10.6209/JORIES.202109_66(3).0006 【Chou, S.-S., & Wang, S.-M. (2021). Relationship between visuospatial working memory and concentration game performance in young children. Journal of Research in Education Sciences, 66(3), 191-212. https://doi.org/10. 6209/JORIES.202109_66(3).0006】
- 洪榮昭、王志美、葉貞妮、吳鳳姝(2020)。遊戲自我效能、遊戲興趣、認知負荷與地理桌遊的遊玩自信心提升之相關研究。教育科學研究期刊,65(3),225-250。https://doi.org/10. 6209/JORIES.202009_65(3).0008【Hong, J.-C., Wang, C.-M., Ye, J.-N., & Wu, F.-S. (2020). The relationship among gameplay self-efficacy, gameplay interest, cognitive load, and self-confidence enhancement in geography board game. Journal of Research in Education Sciences, 65(3), 225-250. https://doi.org/10.6209/JORIES.202009_65(3).0008】
- 徐敏琪、高傳正、王玉華(2021)。原住民幼兒學習數的合成與分解之歷程研究。幼兒教保研究,23,24-52。【Shu, M.-C., Kao, C.-C., & Wang, Y.-H. (2021). A study about the process to learn number composing and decomposing of the indigenous children. Journal of Early Childhood Education & Care, 23, 24-52.】
» More
- 王國亨、簡清華(2008)。屏東縣國小一年級新生的數與計算能力。教育實踐與研究,21(2),1-31。https://doi.org/10.6776/JEPR.200809.0001【Wang, K.-H., & Chien, C.-H. (2008). First graders’ abilities of number and operations in Pingtung County. Journal of Educational Practice and Research, 21(2), 1-31. https://doi.org/10.6776/JEPR.200809.0001】
- 杜雪淇、阮淑宜、林珮伃(2011)。弱勢家庭與非弱勢家庭大班幼兒數學能力之研究。幼兒教育年刊,22,21-41。【Du, S.-C., Juan, S.-Y., & Lin, P.-Y. (2011). Kindergarteners’ mathematics abilities between disadvantaged and advantaged families. Journal of Early Childhood Education, 22, 21-41.】
- 周姍姍、王馨敏(2021)。幼兒視覺空間工作記憶與翻牌遊戲之關聯性研究。教育科學研究期刊,66(3),191-212。https://doi.org/10.6209/JORIES.202109_66(3).0006 【Chou, S.-S., & Wang, S.-M. (2021). Relationship between visuospatial working memory and concentration game performance in young children. Journal of Research in Education Sciences, 66(3), 191-212. https://doi.org/10. 6209/JORIES.202109_66(3).0006】
- 洪榮昭、王志美、葉貞妮、吳鳳姝(2020)。遊戲自我效能、遊戲興趣、認知負荷與地理桌遊的遊玩自信心提升之相關研究。教育科學研究期刊,65(3),225-250。https://doi.org/10. 6209/JORIES.202009_65(3).0008【Hong, J.-C., Wang, C.-M., Ye, J.-N., & Wu, F.-S. (2020). The relationship among gameplay self-efficacy, gameplay interest, cognitive load, and self-confidence enhancement in geography board game. Journal of Research in Education Sciences, 65(3), 225-250. https://doi.org/10.6209/JORIES.202009_65(3).0008】
- 徐敏琪、高傳正、王玉華(2021)。原住民幼兒學習數的合成與分解之歷程研究。幼兒教保研究,23,24-52。【Shu, M.-C., Kao, C.-C., & Wang, Y.-H. (2021). A study about the process to learn number composing and decomposing of the indigenous children. Journal of Early Childhood Education & Care, 23, 24-52.】
- 郭李宗文(2013)。具體化在地化數學操作活動對原住民小一學童數學學習之影響。屏東教育大學學報:教育類,40,183-213。【KuoLi, T.-W. (2013). The influence of materialization and localization of mathematics activities on the indigenous first grade students’ mathematics learning. Journal of Pingtung University of Education: Education, 40, 183- 213.】
- 教育部(2016)。幼兒園教保活動課程大綱。作者。【Ministry of Education. (2016). Curriculum outline of preschool education. Author.】
- 魏培容、郭李宗文、高志誠、高傳正(2011)。新住民與非新住民家庭之幼兒數學能力與語言能力的比較研究。幼兒教育年刊,22,153-171. https://doi.org/10.6475/JECE.201110.0154【Wei, P.-J., KuoLi, T.-W., Kao, C.-C., & Kao, C.-C. (2011). A comparison study on mathematical and language ability of yung children between new immigrant and local families. Journal of Early Childhood Education, 22, 153-171. https://doi.org/10.6475/JECE.201110.0154】
- Baccaglini-Frank, A., & Maracci, M. (2015). Multi-touch technology and preschoolers’ development of number-sense. Digital Experiences in Mathematics Education, 1(1), 7-27. https://doi.org/10. 1007/s40751-015-0002-4
- Behnamnia, N., Kamsin, A., Ismail, M. A. B., & Hayati, A. (2020). The effective components of creativity in digital game-based learning among young children: A case study. Children and Youth Services Review, 116, 105227. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.childyouth.2020.105227
- Berch, D. B. (2005). Making sense of number sense: Implications for children with mathematical disabilities. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(4), 333-339. https://doi.org/10.1177/00222194050380040901
- Booth, J. L., & Siegler, R. S. (2006). Developmental and individual differences in pure numerical estimation. Developmental Psychology, 42(1), 189-201. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649. 41.6.189
- Byun, J., & Joung, E. (2018). Digital game-based learning for K-12 mathematics education: A meta- analysis. School Science & Mathematics, 118(3/4), 113-126. https://doi.org/10.1111/ssm.12271
- Case, R. (1998, April 13-17). A psychological model of number sense [Paper presentation]. The annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, San Diego, CA.
- Clarke, B., Doabler, C. T., Kosty, D., Nelson, E. K., Smolkowski, K., Fien, H., & Baker, S. K. (2017). Testing the efficacy of a kindergarten mathematics intervention by small group size. AERA Open, 3(2), 1-16. https://doi.org/10.1177/2332858417706899
- Clements, D. H. (1984). Training effects on the development and generalization of Piagetian logical operations and knowledge of number. Journal of Educational Psychology, 76(5), 766-776. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.76.5.766
- Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2007). Effects of a preschool mathematics curriculum: Summative research on the building blocks project. Journal for Research in Mathematics Education, 38(2), 136-163. https://doi.org/10.2307/30034954
- Clements, D. H., & Sarama, J. (2011). Early childhood mathematics intervention. Science, 333(6045), 968-970. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1204537
- Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. Oxford University Press.
- Dehaene, S., Bossini, S., & Giraux, P. (1993). The mental representation of parity and numerical magnitude. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 122(3), 371-396. https://doi.org/10. 1037/0096-3445.122.3.371
- Dehaene, S., Piazza, M., Pinel, P., & Cohen, L. (2003). Three parietal circuits for number processing. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3-6), 487-506. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290244000239
- Doabler, C. T., Clarke, B., Kosty, D., Smolkowski, K., Kurtz-Nelson, E., Fien, H., & Baker, S. K. (2019). Building number sense among English learners: A multisite randomized controlled trial of a Tier 2 kindergarten mathematics intervention. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 47, 432-444. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2018.08.004
- Dyson, N. I., Jordan, N. C., & Glutting, J. (2013). A number sense intervention for low-income kindergartners at risk for mathematics difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 46(2), 166-181. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219411410233
- Elofsson, J., Gustafson, S., Samuelsson, J., & Träff, U. (2016). Playing number board games supports 5-year-old children’s early mathematical development. The Journal of Mathematical Behavior, 43, 134-147. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jmathb.2016.07.003
- Frye, D., Baroody, A. J., Burchinal, M., Carver, S. M., Jordan, N. C., & McDowell, J. (2013). Teaching math to young children: A practice guide (NCEE 2014-4005). National Center for Education Evaluation and Regional Assistance, Institute of Education Sciences, U.S. Department of Education.
- Gersten, R., Jordan, N. C., & Flojo, J. R. (2005). Early identification and interventions for students with mathematics difficulties. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(4), 293-304. https://doi.org/10.1177/00222194050380040301
- Ginsburg, H. P., & Golbeck, S. L. (2004). Thoughts on the future of research on mathematics and science learning and education. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19(1), 190-200. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.01.013
- Ginsburg, H. P., Lee, J. S., & Boyd, J. S. (2008). Mathematics education for young children: What it is and how to promote it. Social Policy Report, 22(1), 1-24. https://doi.org/10.1002/j.2379- 3988.2008.tb00054.x
- Griffin, S. A. (2004). Building number sense with number worlds: A mathematics program for young children. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19(1), 173-180. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq. 2004.01.012
- Griffin, S. A., Case, R., & Siegler, R. S. (1994). Rightstart: Providing the central conceptual prerequisites for first formal learning of arithmetic to students at risk for school failure. In K. McGilly (Ed.), Classroom lessons: Integrating cognitive theory and classroom practice (pp. 24-49). MIT Press.
- Halberda, J., & Feigenson, L. (2008). Developmental change in the acuity of the number sense: The approximate number system in 3-, 4-, 5-, and 6-year-olds and adults. Developmental Psychology, 44(5), 1457-1465. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012682
- Hirsch, S., Lambert, K., Coppens, K., & Moeller, K. (2018). Basic numerical competences in large-scale assessment data: Structure and long-term relevance. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 167, 32-48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.09.015
- Huber, B., Tarasuik, J., Antoniou, M. N., Garrett, C., Bowe, S. J., & Kaufman, J. (2016). Young children’s transfer of learning from a touchscreen device. Computers in Human Behavior, 56, 56-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.11.010
- Jordan, N. C., & Dyson, N. (2016). Catching math problems early: Findings from the Number Sense Intervention Project A2 - Henik, Avishai Continuous Issues in Numerical Cognition. Academic Press.
- Jordan, N. C., & Levine, S. C. (2009). Socio-economic variation, number competence, and mathematics learning difficulties in young children. Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 15(1), 60-68. https://doi.org/10.1002/ddrr.46
- Jordan, N. C., Glutting, J., & Dyson, N. (2012). Number Sense Screener (NSS) K-1. Brookes.
- Jordan, N. C., Glutting, J., & Ramineni, C. (2008). A number sense assessment tool for identifying children at risk for mathematical difficulties. In A. Dowker (Ed.), Mathematical difficulties: Psychology and intervention (pp. 45-58). Academic Press.
- Jordan, N. C., Glutting, J., Ramineni, C., & Watkins, M. W. (2010). Validating a number sense screening tool for use in kindergarten and first grade: Prediction of mathematics proficiency in third grade. School Psychology Review, 39(2), 181-195. https://doi.org/10.1080/02796015. 2010.12087772
- Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Locuniak, M. N., & Ramineni, C. (2007). Predicting first-grade math achievement from developmental number sense trajectories. Learning Disabilities Research & Practice, 22(1), 36- 46. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-5826.2007.00229.x
- Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Olah, L., & Locuniak, M. N. (2006). Number sense growth in kindergarten: A longitudinal investigation of children at risk for mathematics difficulties. Child Development, 77(1), 153-175. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2006.00862.x
- Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C., & Locuniak, M. N. (2008). Development of number combination skill in the early school years: When do fingers help? Developmental Science, 11(5), 662-668. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00715.x
- Jordan, N. C., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C., & Locuniak, M. N. (2009). Early math matters: Kindergarten number competence and later mathematics outcomes. Developmental Psychology, 45(3), 850-867. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014939
- Landerl, K., Bevan, A., & Butterworth, B. (2004). Developmental dyscalculia and basic numerical capacities: A study of 8-9-year-old students. Cognition, 93(2), 99-125. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2003.11.004
- Levine, S. C., Jordan, N. C., & Huttenlocher, J. (1992). Development of calculation abilities in young children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 53(1), 72-103. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-0965(05)80005-0
- Li, Q., & Ma, X. (2010). A meta-analysis of the effects of computer technology on school students’ mathematics learning. Educational Psychology Review, 22(3), 215-243. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10648-010-9125-8
- Lin, C.-H., & Chen, C.-M. (2016). Developing spatial visualization and mental rotation with a digital puzzle game at primary school level. Computers in Human Behavior, 57, 23-30. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.12.026
- Link, T., Moeller, K., Huber, S., Fischer, U., & Nuerk, H.-C. (2013). Walk the number line – An embodied training of numerical concepts. Trends in Neuroscience and Education, 2(2), 74-84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tine.2013.06.005
- Locuniak, M. N., & Jordan, N. C. (2008). Using kindergarten number sense to predict calculation fluency in second grade. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 41(5), 451-459. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022219408321126
- Malofeeva, E., Day, J., Saco, X., Young, L., & Ciancio, D. (2004). Construction and evaluation of a number sense test with Head Start children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 96(4), 648-659. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.96.4.648
- Mazzocco, M. M. M. (2005). Challenges in identifying target skills for math disability screening and intervention. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 38(4), 318-323. https://doi.org/10.1177/00222194050380040701
- Moeller, K., Fischer, U., Nuerk, H.-C., & Cress, U. (2015). Computers in mathematics education – Training the mental number line. Computers in Human Behavior, 48, 597-607. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2015.01.048
- National Research Council. (2009). Mathematics learning in early childhood: Paths toward excellence and equity. National Academies Press.
- Papastergiou, M. (2009). Exploring the potential of computer and video games for health and physical education: A literature review. Computers & Education, 53(3), 603-622. https://doi. org/10.1016/j.compedu.2009.04.001
- Patro, K., & Haman, M. (2012). The spatial-numerical congruity effect in preschoolers. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 111(3), 534-542. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.09.006
- Ramani, G. B., & Siegler, R. S. (2008). Promoting broad and stable improvements in low-income children’s numerical knowledge through playing number board games. Child Development, 79(2), 375-394. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01131.x
- Ramani, G. B., & Siegler, R. S. (2011). Reducing the gap in numerical knowledge between low- and middle-income preschoolers. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 32(3), 146-159. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appdev.2011.02.005
- Sarnecka, B. W., Negen, J., & Goldman, M. C. (2018). Early number knowledge indual-language learners form low-SES households. In D. B. Berch, D. C. Geary, & K. M. Koepke (Eds.), Language and culture in mathematical cognition (pp. 225-249). Academic Press.
- Schacter, J., Shih, J., Allen, C. M., DeVaul, L., Adkins, A. B., Ito, T., & Jo, B. (2016). Math shelf: A randomized trial of a prekindergarten tablet number sense curriculum. Early Education and Development, 27(1), 74-88. https://doi.org/10.1080/10409289.2015.1057462
- Siegler, R. S., & Booth, J. L. (2004). Development of numerical estimation in young children. Child Development, 75(2), 428-444. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2004.00684.x
- Siegler, R. S., & Ramani, G. B. (2009). Playing linear number board games-but not circular ones-improves low-income preschoolers’ numerical understanding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101(3), 545-560. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014239
- Starkey, P., Klein, A., & Wakeley, P. (2004). Enhancing young children’s mathematical knowledge through a pre-kindergarten mathematics intervention. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 19(1), 99-120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.01.002
- Sung, Y.-T., Chang, K.-E., & Lee, M. D. (2008). Designing multimedia games for young children’s taxonomic concept development. Computers and Education, 50(3), 1037-1051. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.compedu.2006.07.011
- Van Galen, M., & Reitsma, P. (2008). Developing access to number magnitude: A study of the SNARC effect in 7- to 9-year-olds. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101(2), 99-113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2008.05.001
- Von Aster, M. G., & Shalev, R. S. (2007). Number development and developmental dyscalculia. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 49(11), 868-873. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00868.x
- Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (A. Kozulin ed. & Trans.). MIT Press.
- Whyte, J. C., & Bull, R. (2008). Number games, magnitude representation, and basic number skills in preschoolers. Developmental Psychology, 44(2), 588-596. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012- 1649.44.2.588
- Wilson, A. J., Dehaene, S., Pinel, P., Revkin, S. K., Cohen, L., & Cohen, D. (2006). Principles underlying the design of “The Number Race,” an adaptive computer game for the remediation of dyscalculia. Behavioral and Brain Functions, 2(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.1186/1744- 9081-2-19